Scientists identify link between introspection and brain structure
Our ability to reflect on our own thoughts, emotions and behaviour is one of the key aspects of consciousness and what makes us human, but the biological basis of this process - known as introspection - has, until now, been unknown.
Now, researchers at the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging at University College London (UCL) have identified an area of the brain that is larger in people who are good at introspection, suggesting that this area may play a key role in the process. The study, published in the journal 'Science', was announced today at the British Science Festival in Birmingham.
"We introspect when we think about our own thoughts, feelings or the decisions we have made," says Steve Fleming, joint first author of the study. "It's something we do all the time, but some people are better at it than others. Even if we don't get feedback when we make a choice, we often know intuitively if it's a good or a bad decision."
Measuring introspection has previously proved challenging. Unlike learning, where a person gets better at a task, or decision-making, where we can determine whether a person's choice is correct or not, there are no outward indicators for introspective thought.
To overcome this problem, the research team, led by a Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellow, Professor Geraint Rees from UCL, devised an experiment that could provide both an objective measure of a person's performance at a task and also a measure of how well they could judge their own performance - in other words, how good they were at introspection.
Thirty-two volunteers were each shown two screens. Each screen contained six patches; on one of the screens, one of the patches was brighter than the rest. The volunteers were asked to identify which screen contained the brighter patch, and then asked to rate how confident they were about their decision before they knew whether they were correct.
"We made this task difficult so that people could never be entirely sure about whether their answer was correct," explains Dr Rimona Weil, joint first author on the paper. "Someone who is good at introspection will be confident when they know they are correct, because they have seen it clearly. But they will be less confident when they are not sure whether they are right or wrong."
"It's like 'Who Wants to be a Millionaire?' - a good contestant will go with their answer when they’re sure, and phone a friend if unsure. But a poor contestant might not be as good at judging how likely they are to be correct."
Although all the volunteers performed equally well at the task, introspective ability differed substantially between individuals. The researchers then looked at the structure of the volunteers' brains using scans taken with a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner, looking for parts of the brain that correlated with introspective ability.
"We found a correlation between introspective ability and the structure of a small area of prefrontal cortex near the front of the brain," explains Professor Rees. "The better a person was at introspection, the more grey matter they had in this area. The same was true for the white matter or nerve connections in this area.
"At this stage, we don't know why their grey or white matter differs in this small area. Does this area develop as we get better at reflecting on our thoughts, or are people better at introspection if their prefrontal cortex is more developed in the first place?"
The researchers believe that their findings might help them understand why and how brain damage can affect a person's ability to reflect on their thoughts, and to develop appropriate interventions.
"Take the example of two patients with mental illness, one who is aware of their illness, and one who is not," says Fleming. "The first person is likely to take their medication, the second less likely. If we understand self-awareness at the neurological level, then perhaps we can adapt treatments and develop training strategies for these patients."
This research was funded by Wellcome Trust, the UCL Four-year PhD Programme in Neuroscience and the Medical Research Council.
Reference
Fleming SM et al. Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 16 Sep 2010.
About University College London
Founded in 1826, University College London (UCL), was the first English university established after Oxford and Cambridge, the first to admit students regardless of race, class, religion or gender, and the first to provide systematic teaching of law, architecture and medicine. UCL is the fourth-ranked university in the 2009 THES-QS World University Rankings. UCL alumni include Marie Stopes, Jonathan Dimbleby, Lord Woolf, Alexander Graham Bell, and members of the band Coldplay. UCL currently has over 12,000 undergraduate and 8,000 postgraduate students. Its annual income is over £600 million.
About the Medical Research Council
For almost 100 years the Medical Research Council has improved the health of people in the UK and around the world by supporting the highest quality science. The MRC invests in world-class scientists. It has produced 29 Nobel Prize winners and sustains a flourishing environment for internationally recognised research. The MRC focuses on making an impact and provides the financial muscle and scientific expertise behind medical breakthroughs, including the first antibiotic penicillin, the structure of DNA and the lethal link between smoking and cancer. Today MRC funded scientists tackle research into the major health challenges of the 21st century.
About the Wellcome Trust
The Wellcome Trust is a global charity dedicated to achieving extraordinary improvements in human and animal health. It supports the brightest minds in biomedical research and the medical humanities. The Trust's breadth of support includes public engagement, education and the application of research to improve health. It is independent of both political and commercial interests.